Laserfiche WebLink
<br />908 <br /> <br />Environ. Toxicol. Chern. 20,2001 <br /> <br />were studied for the 96-h exposure (<10, 15.0,30.0,60.0, and <br />120 ILg/L copper). The 266-lLg/L concentration was not used <br />in the 96-h exposure because toxic effects were severe at 24 <br />h, and it was anticipated that too few fish would survive the <br />longer exposure. Hamilton and Buhl [13] reported a 96-h LC50 <br />of 269 ILg/L for Colorado pikeminnow exposed to copper un- <br />der similar water-quality conditions. Concentrations were as- <br />signed to replicate (n = 10) 1-L exposure beakers using a <br />balanced, randomized design. Beakers were filled with test <br />solutions to a depth of about 8.5 cm. Exposure solutions were <br />renewed every 24 h. Five fish were randomly assigned to each <br />exposure beaker. Fish were not fed during chemical exposure. <br />Cool-white fluorescent lamps were the only source of illu- <br />mination, and a 16:8-h light:dark photoperiod was maintained. <br /> <br />Physical and chemical conditions <br /> <br />Dilution water for copper exposures was a mixture of well <br />water and deionized water prepared to match the average hard- <br />ness in July of the Yampa River near Maybell, Colorado, USA. <br />Water quality for this time and location were selected because <br />of correspondence with spawning activity and presence of a <br />spawning site approximately 95 river-km downstream. Aver- <br />age water quality was estimated by calculating mean hardness <br />of two low (1981 and 1989), medium (1980 and 1982), and <br />high (1984 and 1985) water years (data from U.S. Geological <br />Survey gage 09251000; [14-19]). Target water quality char- <br />acteristics were hardness, 124 mg/L as CaC03; alkalinity, 100 <br />mg/L as CaC03; and pH, 8.3. Dilution water was vigorously <br />aerated for approximately 48 h while being maintained at the <br />test temperature of 20 2: 20C. Dissolved oxygen, hardness, pH, <br />alkalinity, and specific conductance were measured daily dur- <br />ing toxicant exposures. Water temperature was measured con- <br />tinuously with a temperature recorder. Means and ranges for <br />the measured dilution water characteristics were dissolved ox- <br />ygen, 6.9 (6.5-7.5) mg/L; hardness, 117 (113-120) mg/L as <br />CaC03; pH, 8.3 (8.1-8.5); alkalinity, 80 (71-88) mg/L as <br />CaC03; specific conductance, 240 ILS/cm, and temperature, <br />18.5 to 21.0oC. <br /> <br />Copper solutions and analytical procedures <br /> <br />Stock solutions of analytical-grade copper sulfate (Mal- <br />linckrodt, Paris, KY, USA) were prepared in deionized water. <br />Exposure concentrations were prepared by pipetting the de- <br />sired amount of toxicant stock into 1-L glass beakers con- <br />taining 0.75 L dilution water. Exposure solutions were stirred <br />and transferred to exposure beakers within 30 min of prepa- <br />ration. <br />Toxicant concentrations were measured at the beginning of <br />exposure periods. On each occasion, two 50-ml samples were <br />collected from separate beakers for each exposure concentra- <br />tion. Unfiltered samples were placed in acid-washed polyeth- <br />ylene bottles, acidified to pH < 2 with analytical-grade nitric <br />acid, and held at 40C until analyzed by inductively coupled <br />plasma emission spectroscopy (Soil, Water, and Plant Testing <br />Laboratory, Colorado State University, Fort Collins, CO, <br />USA). Chemical analysis confirmed accuracy of exposure con- <br />centrations. Measured copper concentrations averaged 92% <br />(standard error = 4.8) of nominal. Measured and nominal con- <br />centrations were in close agreement; consequently, statistical <br />analyses were based on nominal concentrations. <br /> <br />Behavioral assay <br /> <br />Skin homogenate that was assumed to contain fright-pher- <br />omone was prepared before each assay. Donor Colorado <br /> <br />D.W. Beyers and M.S. Farmer <br /> <br />pikeminnow were juveniles obtained from the same mass cul- <br />ture used for study fish. Homogenate preparation involved <br />removing skin posterior to the head and anterior to insertion <br />of the caudal fin. Skin was cut into pieces and homogenized <br />(Omni-mixer, Sorvall, Norwalk, CT, USA) in dilution water <br />to a final concentration of 12.5 g/L. <br />The acclimation period for each behavioral assay was ini- <br />tiated immediately after conclusion of the 24- or 96-h expo- <br />sures. Fish and solutions from each replicate were transferred <br />to flow-through observation aquaria receiving dilution water <br />at a rate of 50 ml/min. Observation aquaria were lOX 20 X <br />15 cm high, and depth of water was 12 cm. Each aquarium <br />was enclosed within a blind to visually isolate fish. An ob- <br />servation port allowed access for a video camera lens. Fish <br />were allowed to acclimate to observation aquaria for approx- <br />imately 90 min before being tested for presence of a fright <br />reaction. After the acclimation period, movements of fish in <br />each aquarium were video recorded for 1 min, followed by <br />introduction of skin homogenate and an additional 1 min of <br />video recording. Aliquots (0.1 ml) of skin homogenate were <br />introduced into observation aquaria through injection ports in <br />the water-delivery system, and final concentration of the ho- <br />mogenate in aquaria was 0.625 mg/L. Preliminary studies in <br />which dilution water was injected into the delivery system <br />showed that the procedure did not disturb the fish or elicit a <br />fright reaction. Behavioral assays were conducted so that per- <br />sons responsible for video camera operation and fright-pher- <br />omone injections were unaware (blind) of the experimental <br />treatment assigned to fish within each aquarium. Upon con- <br />clusion of behavioral assays, three fish from each experimental <br />treatment were preserved for SEM observations and the re- <br />mainder were returned to culture facilities. Remaining fish <br />from the 96-h exposure were cultured for 14 d, then were <br />reassayed using the procedure described above to evaluate the <br />potential for recovery of olfactory ability. <br /> <br />.( <br /> <br />.J <br />I <br />II <br /> <br />Video interpretation <br /> <br />The behavioral reaction to fright pheromone is facilitated <br />by social interactions between fish. Consequently, the com- <br />bined response of all fish within an observation aquarium (ex- <br />perimental unit) was the unit of measurement. Generally, un- <br />disturbed fish were dispersed and oriented in different direc- <br />tions within an aquarium and only occasionally changed their <br />position during the 1-min preexposure observation period. Cri- <br />teria used to identify a positive fright reaction response were <br />demonstration of one or more of the following behaviors: (1) <br />cover seeking, characterized by fish assembling into a polar- <br />ized school, then moving toward the bottom of the aquarium <br />and reducing movement; (2) agitation, characterized by rapid <br />movement and frequent turning; and (3) dashing, characterized <br />by one or more fish displaying frenzied behavior, including <br />jumping out of the water, repeated and rapid changes in di- <br />rection, and swimming against aquarium walls. The criteria <br />were used only to qualify presence or absence of a fright <br />reaction and not to quantify intensity of the response. <br />Video interpretation was conducted by an observer who <br />was unaware (blind) of the experimental treatment assigned <br />to fish within each aquarium. The criteria were used to interpret <br />behavior during the first minute of each 2-min recording in- <br />terval in order to identify fish that were likely to give a fa1se- <br />positive response. Fish that demonstrated cover seeking, ag- <br />itation, or dashing behaviors before introduction of the skin <br />homogenate were excluded from subsequent analysis. Presence <br /> <br />, <br />.. <br />