Laserfiche WebLink
<br />FISH CULTURE-PERSPECTIVE <br /> <br />has a strong research and evaluation component. In spite <br />of these efforts, data for the early 1990s showed that, com- <br />pared to natural adults, returning hatchery fish were <br />younger, were smaller for the same age, and had lower <br />fecundity for the same size (Bugert et al. 1992). The under- <br />lying causes of these somewhat surprising phenotypic <br />changes are not known; however, even if the changes were <br />entirely an environmental response to hatchery conditions, <br />they still would represent a significant single-generation <br />reduction in productivity of the population. <br />· Reisenbichler (1997) has compiled available informa- <br />tion regarding the effects of hatchery culture on fitness of <br />anadromous Pacific salmonids in the wild. These data-all <br />from controlled experimental studies-are imperfect: They <br />apply to a single species (steelhead) and in some cases <br />confound the effects of fish culture and stock transfers. <br />Nevertheless, they are the best available data on this issue, <br />and they demonstrate that fitness of hatchery-reared fish <br />in the wild can be substantially reduced compared with <br />natural fish. Furthermore, the reductions in fitness <br />occurred across all life stages and increased with the num- <br />ber of generations of fish culture. <br />These examples are important but not entirely satisfy- <br />ing; they raise as many questions as they answer. Never- <br />theless, they emphasize a point made by Busack and Cur- <br />rens (1995:77) regarding effects of hatchery fish on natural <br />populations: "We are unaware of rigorous research <br />designed to detect genetic impacts that has failed to find <br />them." What is lacking is a consensus on what constitutes <br />a reasonable approach to this issue given the substantial <br />uncertainty involved and the potentially major conse- <br />quences of whatever actions are (or are not) taken. <br /> <br />Myth 5: It is not a hatchery problem; it is a fisheries <br />management problem. <br />Campton (1995) and others (e.g., Rense11997) have <br />argued that critics of hatcheries often confound biological <br />factors intrinsic to hatcheries with effects of fisheries man- <br />agement (stock transfers, selective breeding, extensive har- <br />vest rates in mixed-stock fisheries). As a result, many <br />hatchery managers believe they have "become scapegoats <br />for virtually every perceived negative biological effect associ- <br />ated with the artificial propagation, release, and management <br />of anadromous salmonid fish" (Campton 1995:338). This <br />argument has some merit. Adaptive management is most <br />effective when the consequences of individual actions can <br />be evaluated and modified as appropriate. To the extent that <br />distinguishing between biological and management fac- <br />tors facilitates this process, it can be useful and productive. <br />However, we should be careful not to exaggerate the <br />dichotomy between biology and management. No fish <br />hatchery exists in a vacuum isolated from fisheries man- <br />agement concerns; rather, every hatchery program is <br />designed to meet one or more management objectives <br />(e.g., harvest enhancement, mitigation, conservation). <br />Some factors identified by Campton such as stock trans- <br />fers and mixed-stock fisheries are primarily a function of <br />fisheries management rather than fish culture, but many <br /> <br />18 . Fisheries <br /> <br />others involve both. For example, selective breeding, when <br />it occurs, is carried out by fish culturists to achieve a fish- <br />eries management objective. The effects on natural popula- <br />tions from an action such as selective breeding are the <br />same whether one chooses to allocate this action to fish <br />culture or fisheries management. In this case, the only <br />meaningful unit to consider is the overall hatchery pro- <br />gram, which encompasses both fish culture and fisheries <br />management. Put another way, "Fish culture is as much a <br />part of management as management is of fish culture" <br />(Incerpi 1996). <br />It is unfortunate if hatchery managers and fish cultur- <br />ists believe they are scapegoats in the controversies about <br />hatcheries. The concerns about hatcheries I have raised <br />here and elsewhere are not directed at those involved in <br />the culture of fish-professionals conscientiously trying to <br />do their job the best way they know how-but rather <br />toward the effects of hatchery programs on natural fish <br />populations. To the extent that we can depersonalize the <br />debate about hatcheries and redirect energy to solving the <br />problems rather than trying to assess blame, the resource <br />as a whole will benefit. <br /> <br />Myth 6: Everything will be okay if we have a good <br />monitoring program. <br />Although no one doubts the importance of monitoring <br />and evaluation (M&E) for adaptive management, there is a <br />very real danger in relying too heavily on M&E as a sub- <br />stitute for meaningful and comprehensive risk manage- <br />ment. The danger arises from three factors. <br />First, M&E for hatchery programs has limitations with <br />respect to both reliability and timeliness. Statistical power <br />to detect genetic effects of hatcheries can be relatively high <br />for selectively neutral molecular markers, which can pro- <br />vide key information on gene flow between hatchery and <br />natural populations, individual reproductive success, and <br />effective population size (e.g., Waples et al. 1993). Howev- <br />er, the most serious concerns regarding fish hatcheries in- <br />volve fitness effects on phenotypic, behavioral, and life <br />history traits in natural populations. As discussed by Hard <br />(1995; see also Peterman 1990), the power of even the most <br />ambitious M&E program to statistically detect a fish cul- <br />ture effect on traits such as these is likely to be very low <br />because the background noise level (natural variability in <br />the same traits) is typically very high. Furthermore, even if <br />such an effect is detected, it will generally occur only after <br />several (fish) generations of monitoring. This means that <br />artificial propagation could substantially harm natural <br />populations long before there is any reasonable expectation <br />of being able to detect it. Of course, this does not mean that <br />deleterious effects will always occur; however, it should <br />give pause to those willing to embark on a high-risk pro- <br />gram in the expectation that they will be able to quickly <br />and surgically intervene at the first sign of an undesirable <br />outcome. Unfortunately, in most cases that is a myth. <br />Second, even if harm is found, there is no guarantee <br />that effective remedial action will be taken. To be effective, <br />an adaptive management program should include a <br /> <br />Vol. 24, No.2 <br />