My WebLink
|
Help
|
About
|
Sign Out
Home
Browse
Search
7707
CWCB
>
UCREFRP
>
Copyright
>
7707
Metadata
Thumbnails
Annotations
Entry Properties
Last modified
7/14/2009 5:01:45 PM
Creation date
5/20/2009 1:33:28 PM
Metadata
Fields
Template:
UCREFRP
UCREFRP Catalog Number
7707
Author
Saul, G. E.
Title
Effects of repetitive electroshocking on fish populations in experimental raceways and a small headwater stream in southern West Virginia
USFW Year
1981
USFW - Doc Type
41(12)
Copyright Material
YES
There are no annotations on this page.
Document management portal powered by Laserfiche WebLink 9 © 1998-2015
Laserfiche.
All rights reserved.
/
2
PDF
Print
Pages to print
Enter page numbers and/or page ranges separated by commas. For example, 1,3,5-12.
After downloading, print the document using a PDF reader (e.g. Adobe Reader).
Show annotations
View images
View plain text
<br />.. 4320-B, <br /> <br /> <br />q~l <br /> <br />AGRICULTURE, Al\1MAL CULTURE Al\U ~'UfRITION <br /> <br />() '17tJ <br /> <br />ASSOClA nON OF GROWfH AND ENDOCRINE FUNCIlON IN <br />TIIE PREPUBERTAL BEEF CALF: L AGE AND SEX <br />DIFFEREJ\crs IN PITUITARY RESPONSIVEl\'F.SS TO GNRH. <br />n. AGE AND SEASON DIFFERENCES IN NEGATIVE <br />FEEDBACK MECH.AI\1SMS REGULATING lJJ SECJU:llON. <br />In. RELA TIONSHlP OF GROWTH AND PUBERTY IN BEEF <br />HEIFERS FED MO!\'ENSIN Order No. 8111610 <br />MOSElJn', WIlJ..LUf MICHAEL.. PH.D. University qfWyoming,1980. UOpp. <br />Two uperiment$ were COI1l1\1cte4 \0 c1etennint lie and sex cWrerlUlccs : <br />Dirsenation Abstracts InterTUltionaJ Vol ~1 No. J] JU1l8 1981 <br /> <br />same foundation stock.. Suain 5 is a randombred control and Strain 3 bad <br />been selected for early-year egg production in intenningled-straiD floor <br />pens for 15 years and subsequently in single-bird cages for five years. Three <br />laying-house environments involved in Experiment 1 were: single-bird <br />cages, separated-strain floor pens. and intermingled-strain floor pens. In <br />addition. Experiment 2 also included a thr~bird cage environment There <br />were 360 and 384 pullets per strain involved in Experiments 1 and 2. <br />respea.ively. <br />In order to measure the social status of pullets within flocks, a social <br />rank index (X) was devised. It is defineel as: X = Ih(D - S + N + 1). where <br />D = number dominated. S = number dominating. and N = group size. <br />Behavioral data obtained from eight separated-strain fiocks in Experiment 2 <br />were analyzed to stuely the properties of the social rank index anel its <br />relationships with other measures of agonistic behavior and.with <br />productivity traits. <br />Social rank index appeared to be a better measurement of individual <br />social statuS than social tension index (total aggressive acts min.us total <br />submissive acts), because it tended to be more normally distributed.. <br />Regression of the frequency of agonistic behaviors on social rank indei <br />indicat.ed that most agonistic interactions occurring in the flock were <br />associated with pullets near the top of the peck order delivering aggressive <br />acts towarel those near the bottom of the peck order. <br />Comparisons of the linear and three curvilinear regression moc1els <br />indicated a definite tendency toward curvilinear relationships between an <br />individual's social statUS and its productivity within the flock.. The egg <br />proc1uction of pullets at or near the bottom of the peck order was <br />disproportionately depressed. <br />Both experiments indicated that pullets of selected Suain 3 had social <br />dominance over control Suain 5 pullets when five months olel (before <br />sexual mawrity). but domin:mte by Suain 3 females diminished or <br />disappeared when strangers of the two strains were placed together when <br />17 months old. Nevertheless, because ofsocial inertia, Strain 3 pullets <br />maintained undiminished dominance over Strain 5 pullets for the entire <br />laying year when individuals were kept together from housing time onward. <br />Suain 3 pullets had more aggressive acts. more feeding activity. and <br />spent less time resting than did Strain 5 pullets in both separated- and <br />intenningled-strain housing environments. When pullets of the two strains <br />were kept together. the relative frequencies of their agonistic behaviors <br />were dwlged, but nonagonistic activities were not altered. <br />Genotype by environment interactions were detected as being <br />significant in Experiment 1 for egg and body weights, and in Experiment 2 <br />for rate of lay. livability, egg weight, hen-housed rate of lay, egg mass, body <br />weight, and body weight gain. Although fewer genotype by en\ironment <br />interactions were found in Experiment 1. there was a distinct tendency for <br />the suains to differ (or fail to differ) in similar ways in each of the three <br />environments compared in the two experiments. <br />Pullets of the more aggressive anel socially dominant selected Strain 3 <br />were generally superior in performance to unselected Strain 5 pullets when <br />they were intermingled in floor pens, but those differences tended to <br />disappear when pullets of the two strains were kept in separated-strain <br />fiocks. These results are taken as indirect evidence supporting the <br />conclusion that differences in stress associated with the social environment <br />may contribute to genotype by housing environment interactions for <br />performance traits. <br />linear and curvilinear regression analyses for pullets in single-bird <br />cages indicated that selected Suain 3 indivieluals were. on the average. near <br />their optimal body weights, but unselected control Suain 5 pullets were <br />overweight under conditions of unlimited feed availability. <br /> <br />I!\'FLUENCE OF DIETARY MODIFICATIONS ON TIlE <br />INCIDE.~CE OF FATrY UVER HEMORRHAGIC SYNDROME IN <br />JAPANESE QUAIL A.l\'D LAYING HENS Order No. 8108828 <br />M.+. TOO. D.1l.MENcrr A DIR.ECTO, PH.D. University of M WoUn - Columbia. <br />1980. 192pp. <br />Abstract Not Available. <br /> <br /> <br />in the feedback mech~ni"'".S regulating luteinizing hormone secrelion of the . <br />prepubertal calf. Another experiment was designed to determine whether <br />feeding monensin had an effect on age and weight at puberty which is <br />inelependent of increased body weight gains. <br />Age and sex differences in pituitaIy responsiveness of prepubertal beef <br />calves to gonadotropin releasing hormone (GnRH) were determined in the <br />.first study. A single injection of 125 p.g GnRH induced a sigr>ificantrise in <br />serum luteinizing hormone (UI) concentrations in fall and SPrin& born <br />heifers at 60 and 200 days of age and in fall bom bulls at 100 and 200 days <br />of age. The magnitude of the Ul peak. duration of the Ul peak and.area of <br />Ul response curve was similar between ages and season ofbinh in heifers. <br />D.uation of the ill peak and area of ill response curve were grea1er in <br />lQO-day-old bulls than 200-day-old bulls and heifers at bothaaes. The <br />magnitude of the ill peak was greater in bulls than heifers.. <br />A second study was designed to examine the effectSo( age and sea50lI of <br />birth on the response of the hypothalamic:-pituitaIy-gonadalaxis 10. the <br />negative feedback. action of estradiol in the prepubertal beefhei(er. Fall <br />and spring born heifers, 60 and 200 days of age, were assigned 10 on&: of <br />three treatment groups: (1) control-intact; (2) ovariectomy; or. .... <br />(3) ovariectomy plus estradiol-17fJ <EU arlmini~('led in si1astic implants. <br />ill was measured in serum samples collected at 2O-min intervals for 4 <br />hours the day before and 7. 2l. 35 and.49 days after ovarieaomy. Removal <br />of the ovaries resulted in elevated serum UI concentrations at both 60 and <br />200 days of age. A transition was made from basal Ul with a low ~uency <br />of pulsatile LH secretion prior to ovariectOmy to a panero characterized by <br />rllythmic LH pulses at hourlyinteIVals by .49 days after ovariecUmy. The <br />inhibition of the post-ovariectomy rise in serum ill by E2 replacement <br />therapy demonstrateS that ~ exerts negative feedback on pituiwy UI <br />secretion in the prepubertal beef heifer. Mean serum E2 concentrations <br />were three times greater than oontrol or ovariectomized heifers but the <br />latter were similar. Seasonal variation in the negative feedback action of~ <br />on ill secretion may exist in 200-day-old but not in 6O-day-oldheifers. ... <br />In the final experiment one hundred forty crossbred beefheifers were <br />allotted to either a heavy (H) or light (L) group consisting of heifers with . <br />above and below the average weaning weights, respectively. Within weight <br />class, heifers were assigned to receive one of three feed treatments for 2D3 <br />days: (1) roughage diet (R) fed SO% roughage and 20% concentrate; (2) R <br />plus monensin (Ml) fed to proc1uce same daily gains as R by reducing feed <br />intake 10%; (3) R plus monensin (M2) fed the same daily feed intake as R <br />but proc1ucing increased daily gains. ADO was greater (P < .001) in M2 than <br />Rand Ml, but the latter twO were similar. Monensin increased (P < .01) <br />rumina! propionic acid production while acectic and butyric acid were <br />decreased (P < .01). Within L heifers, condition scores were greater (p< .01) <br />in the M1 and M2 groups compared with R. In H heifers. condition score <br />was greater (P < .01) for the M2 group than either Ml or R. Hip height was <br />not affected by treatment, but H heifers were taller (p < .01) than L heifers. <br />During the feeding period, 92% of M2L and 100% of all other groUps <br />reached puberty. In the L group, pubertal age and weight were similar <br />across treatments. However. in the H group. M1 anel M2 heifers were 14 <br />days younger (P < .06) at puberty than R heifers and M2 heifers were <br />heavier (P < .01) at puberty than Ml and R heifers.. Pregnancy rates were <br />not different between treatments. In conclusion. monensin decreased ageal <br />puberty indepenelent of increased ADO or body weight; however, tl1is <br />effect was only present in the H group. Itappears that H heifers possess an <br />inherently greater growth Potential than L heifers, and this increase cannot <br />be explained by differences in body weight . <br /> <br />EFFECTS OF REPE1lTIVE ELF.(:TROSH~G O~ ~H <br />POPlJ'LHIOXS IX' EXPERl\1E.'TAL RACEWAYS .-\.'1) _\ <br />S!\1ALL HE.U>WATER STREA.\1 ~ SOl.'nIER.~ WEST <br />VlRGL'\1A Order ='0. 8110474 <br />SAL'l.., GARY Ew., PH.D. Yirgini4 Polytechnic Institute and SIiJl( . <br />Univo:riry,1980. 152pp.. .,. .. <br />FIsheries scientists have utilized electrofishing extensively to sample anQ <br />assess aquatic populations. The physiological responsesoffish~posed to <br />electroshock ranges from sensor)' recognition. through electrotaxis and <br />electronarcosis, to death. This study examined the etfects of repetiti\'e <br />electroshocking over time on population survival, and the etfects of <br />mortality on the estimation of population size using single. .tenS\ilS.. multiple <br />census and depletion techniques. .. u' .. <br />Highly significant differences (P < 0.01) were seen ben;een the number <br />of shocks a population of fathead minnows (Pimephales promeJas) received <br />per day and the survi\'al of those fish. Populations recehing the two control <br />
The URL can be used to link to this page
Your browser does not support the video tag.