Laserfiche WebLink
KARP AND TYUS-COLORADO SQUAWFISH INTERACTION 27 <br />2400 h. A single trial (e.g., 0600 h, 1200 h, <br />1600 h, 2400 h) was composed of fish position <br />data taken at 0, 1, 2, 4, 8, and 16 min time <br />intervals for all species. Data collection began <br />2 min after the observer entered the laboratory <br />so that fish presumably had time to resume un- <br />disturbed activities. An additional 15 sec lag <br />time was allowed at 1200 h and 2400 h due to <br />logistics of feeding. This sequence (0600 h-2400 <br />h) was repeated at least six times for each species <br />over a 9 d period. <br />Aggression and activity patterns of all species <br />were evaluated by recording agonistic interac- <br />tions and movements (passage from 1 cell to <br />another) of randomly selected fish (i.e., individ- <br />ual chosen for observation was that fish closest <br />to some point in the tank taken from a random <br />number table). These data were collected in <br />three dimensional space (36 cell matrix) during <br />2 min periods, and sampling was with replace- <br />ment. We recorded aggression and activity ob- <br />servations following collection of the fish posi- <br />tiondata. Atotal of 33 individuals of each species <br />in tanks 1-4 was observed for 132 min, 51 P. <br />lucius and 47 A. melas (tank 5) were observed <br />for 196 min, and 51 P. lucius and 53 L. cyanellus <br />(tank 6) were observed for 208 min. Nips, chas- <br />es, and threats were the only forms of agonistic <br />behavior observed. We considered nips and <br />chases to be intentional aggressive acts. Chases <br />were sustained charges resulting in an obvious <br />displacement of the recipient. Threats were <br />considered to be instantaneous non-physical <br />confrontations which may or may not .have <br />caused displacement of one or both individuals. <br />We were not able to detect overt aggression in <br />"threat" encounters and so determined these <br />to be the least aggressive of all agonistic acts <br />observed. <br />Following completion of the space use, activ- <br />ity, and agonistic behavior observations, species <br />were separated into monospecific groups. Each <br />group was offered 10, 13-25 mm hatchery- <br />reared razorback suckers, Xyrauchen texanus <br />(from Dexter National Fish Hatchery, Dexter, <br />New Mexico), and all acts of aggression and <br />predation were recorded in the following 60 <br />min period. <br />Some of the initial fish captures died from <br />infections of "white-spot disease" Ichthyophthiri- <br />us mult~liis (D. E. Anderson, pers. comm.), and <br />all subsequent collections were treated with a <br />solution of 0.1 ppm malachite green + 25 ppm <br />formalin immediately following capture. We <br />held fishes in partitioned aquaria in monospecif- <br />is groups for at least 2 mo prior to experimen- <br />tation, and no further disease outbreaks were <br />noted. <br />Data analyses.-Fish position data was summed <br />vertically by layer (upper/middle/lower strat- <br />ification of the water column) by sample period <br />(e.g., 0600 h, 1200 h, 1600 h, 2400 h) for all <br />species. We used a XY contingency table analysis <br />(Sokal and Rohlf, 1981) to compare day, night, <br />feeding and nonfeeding distribution patterns, <br />and to investigate potential shifts in vertical space <br />use by P. lucius when in the presence of N. lu- <br />trensis, R. balteatus, and Pimephales promelas. Ag- <br />onistic behaviors per minute of observation were <br />tabulated for each species by sampling period. <br />Goodness of fit Chi-square tests, X2 (Sokal and <br />Rohlf, 1981), were used to evaluate aggression <br />between nonfeeding and feeding periods for all <br />species. We evaluated activity rates (i.e., average <br />number of total cell changes per minute obser- <br />vation) and movements (i.e., average number of <br />different cells used per minute of observation) <br />using two sample t-tests, Student's t (Sokal and <br />Rohlf, 1981), for diel and species differences. <br />Vertical distribution patterns, aggression and <br />activity levels of Ptychocheilus lucius in tanks 5 <br />and 6 were evaluated within each tank and <br />ranked for an overall view of species differ- <br />ences, but were not statistically compared with <br />the behavior of P. lucius in the C. latipinnis tank <br />because of differing densities. <br />RESULTS <br />Distribution patterns. -Ptychocheilus lucius exhib- <br />itedsome variability in vertical distribution with <br />respect to time of day and presence of food, but <br />generally were dispersed throughout the water <br />column in all six tanks (Fig. 1). The vertical <br />distribution of P. lucius in the tank containing <br />C. latipinnis was significantly different from those <br />in the N. lutrensis tank (x2 = 20.914, df = 2, P <br />< 0.05), Pimephales promelas tank (X2 = 9.591, <br />df = 2, P < 0.05), and R. balteatus tank (X2 = <br />11.339, df = 2, P < 0.05). Vertical distribution <br />of Ptychocheilus lucius was most similar to that of <br />L. cyanellus and R. balteatus (Fig. 2), but all fishes <br />exhibited a distribution pattern that was signif- <br />icantly different from that of P. lucius in the C. <br />latipinnis tank (all Xg > 7.014, df = 2, P < 0.05). <br />The C. latipinnis, A. melas, N. lutrensis, and Pi- <br />mephales promelas spent less time in the upper <br />portion of the tank and exhibited less spatial <br />overlap with Ptychocheilus lucius. <br />