Laserfiche WebLink
646 <br />MESA AND SCHRECK <br />each observation interval, the diver counted the <br />number of marked fish that seemingly behaved <br />normally (hereafter referred to as normal fish) and <br />marked fish that behaved abnormally (abnormal <br />fish), as determined by comparisons with the be- <br />havior of undisturbed cutthroat trout. We also <br />snorkeled outside the section boundary to assess <br />the extent of fish emigration. We plotted the per- <br />centage of marked cutthroat trout that behaved <br />normally or abnormally or that were not observed <br />over time to determine the length of time required <br />for half of the marked fish to return to the normal <br />preshock behavioral condition. <br />In 1988, six sections (some reused from the pre- <br />vious year; Table 1) were subjected to a multiple- <br />electroshock protocol. At each section, after block <br />seines were placed, two divers counted the num- <br />ber of cutthroat trout longer than 100 mm. One <br />diver swam upstream and counted the fish in cov- <br />er and out of cover; the second diver followed <br />after the first diver had snorkeled about 75% of <br />the length of the section. After completion of both <br />fish surveys, we made a single-pass electrofishing <br />run, as previously described. Captured fish were <br />held in buckets along the stream bank. Immedi- <br />ately after the first pass, the divers again snorkeled <br />along the section, in reversed order of entry, to <br />count fish located in and out of cover. This se- <br />quence continued for a maximum of three elec- <br />trofishing passes. (Equipment failed at section 10 <br />midway through the protocol, and data from that <br />section could not be used.) The counts of fish after <br />each electrofishing pass were compared to note <br />any changes in the proportions of fish in and out <br />of cover in relation to those seen in the initial dive. <br />Artificial stream observations. -We conducted <br />laboratory tests with an oval stream aquarium that <br />had recirculating water (Reeves et al. 1983). The <br />stream was 4.3 x 4.9 m on the sides, 0.76 m wide, <br />and 0.61 m deep. It was filled to varying depths <br />with gravel and cobble substrates to produce a <br />stream with four pools (50 cm deep) and three <br />riffles (40 cm deep). A perforated feeding tube along <br />the stream bottom evenly distributed food to sim- <br />ulate insect drift. Each pool contained hollow tiles <br />or stacked bricks to provide additional shelter. <br />Lighting, provided by nine 60-W incandescent <br />bulbs spaced evenly above the stream channel, <br />was controlled by a timer (Everest and Rodgers <br />1982) that provided a graded-intensity photope- <br />riod of 12 h light : 12 h darkness. We maintained <br />water temperature with a cooling and heating unit <br />set at 12-13°C, and water velocity (0.0-10.0 cm/ <br />s) with a rotating paddle wheel. Water was con- <br />tinually passed through a sand filter and ultravi- <br />olet-light sterilizer; make-up water was added to <br />the channel at 0.5 L/min. A curtain with screened <br />windows surrounded the inside perimeter of the <br />stream to permit observation of the fish without <br />disturbing them. Fish were fed frozen brine shrimp <br />thawed in collecting tanks and then passed through <br />the feeding tube to simulate drift. We conducted <br />six trials: three with hatchery-reared cutthroat trout <br />(average weight ± SE, 63 ± 2.7 g) and three with <br />wild fish (46 ± 5.7 g) captured by angling in local <br />streams with artificial flies and barbless hooks. <br />The stream was drained, sterilized, refilled, and <br />stocked with new fish between trials, which were <br />conducted from May to July 1988. <br />For each trial, seven cutthroat trout were al- <br />lowed to acclimate to the stream for up to 2 weeks <br />(wild fish often required the longer time). We <br />judged acclimation by the sustained presence of <br />active feeding and aggressive behavior that led to <br />the formation of a dominance hierarchy. During <br />this period, we fed the fish twice daily (at various <br />times from 0800 to 1000 hours and 1500 to 1700 <br />hours) and observed their behavior. <br />After the acclimation period, we conducted fo- <br />cal-animal sampling (Altmann 1974) during feed- <br />ing sessions for three consecutive days. Each fish <br />was observed for 5 min during which the number <br />of feeding bites taken and the number of aggres- <br />sive acts elicited and received were recorded. The <br />number of aggressive acts consisted of a sum of <br />the individual agonistic elements of salmonids, <br />including nips, charges, chases, and lateral and <br />frontal displays, as described by Kalleberg (1958), <br />Keenleyside and Yamamoto (1962), and Hartman <br />(1965). The sequence in which fish were observed <br />was randomized for each feeding session. At 0800 <br />hours on the fourth day, fish were captured by <br />electrofishing (Coffelt model II-A or Smith-Root <br />model VII backpack units set at 300 V DC) and <br />then marked as described for the natural stream <br />observations. At release, the location and behav- <br />ioral state of each fish was recorded. Feeding and <br />aggression data were recorded by use of focal-an- <br />imal sampling for 2 min each hour during hours <br />1-7 and once 24 h after release. Because the ob- <br />servations were made each hour, we reduced ob- <br />servation time to maximize the time interval be- <br />tween observations and minimize any effect of <br />satiation later in the day. All data were converted <br />to events per fish per minute for comparison. <br />We calculated mean feeding and aggression rates <br />for each fish from the pretreatment data. We then <br />calculated a grand mean for each trial from the