Laserfiche WebLink
<br />Transactions of the American Fisheries Society l 18:644-658, 1989 <br />e 770 <br />Electrofishing Mark-Recapture and Depletion Methodologies <br />Evoke Behavioral and Physiological Changes <br />in Cutthroat Trout <br />MATTHEW G. MESA' AND CARL B. SCHRECK <br />U.S. Fish and Wildlife Service, Oregon Cooperative Fishery Research Unite <br />Department of Fisheries and Wildlife, Oregon State University <br />Corvallis, Oregon 97331, USA <br />Abstract. -We examined the behavioral and physiological responses of wild and hatchery-reared <br />cutthroat trout Oncorhynchus clarki subjected to a single electroshock, electroshock plus marking, <br />and multiple electroshocks in natural and artificial streams. In a natural stream, cutthroat trout <br />released after capture by electrofishing and marking showed distinct behavioral changes: fish im- <br />mediately sought cover, remained relatively inactive, did not feed, and were easily approached by <br />a diver. An average of 3-4 h was required for 50% of the fish to return to a seemingly normal <br />mode of behavior, although responses varied widely among collection sites. Using the depletion <br />method, we observed little change in normal behavior of fish remaining in the stream section (i.e., <br />uncaptured fish) after successive passes with electrofishing gear. In an artificial stream, hatchery- <br />reared and wild cutthroat trout immediately decreased their rates of feeding and aggression after <br />they were electroshocked and marked. Hatchery fish generally recovered in 2-3 h; wild fish required <br />at least 24 h to recover. Analysis of feeding and aggression data by hierarchical rank revealed no <br />distinct recovery trends among hatchery fish of different ranks; among wild cutthroat trout, how- <br />ever, socially dominant fish seemed to recover faster than intermediate and subordinate fish. <br />Physiological indicators of stress (plasma cortisol and blood lactic acid) increased significantly in <br />cutthroat trout subjected to electroshock plus marking or single or multiple electroshocks. As <br />judged by the magnitude of the greatest change in cortisol and lactate, multiple electroshocks <br />elicited the most severe stress response; however, plasma concentrations of both substances had <br />returned to unstressed control levels by 6 h after treatment. It was evident that electrofishing and <br />the procedures involved with estimating fish population size elicited a general stress response that <br />was manifested not only physiologically but also behaviorally. These responses may affect the <br />accuracy of population size estimates by violating key assumptions of the methods, especially the <br />assumption of equal catchability of fish. <br />Because electrofishing is widely used to collect <br />fish for various purposes, the effects of electricity <br />on fish have received much attention. The phys- <br />iological effects of electricity on fish include a va- <br />riety of sublethal changes in blood chemistry that <br />last for various lengths of time (Schreck et al. 1976; <br />Bouck et al. 1978; van Waarde and Kesbeke 1983). <br />Morphological effects include physical injury and <br />sometimes death; however, many studies of the <br />lethality or incidence of injury due to electroshock <br />have been conducted in unnatural situations and <br />often under severe electrical conditions (Spencer <br />1967; Whaley et al. 1978; Hudy 1985). Studies of <br />the effects of electricity on fish behavior have con- <br />' Present address: U.S. Fish and Wildlife Service, Co- <br />lumbia River Field Station, Star Route, Cook, Wash- <br />ington 98605, USA. <br />2 Cooperators are Oregon State University, Oregon <br />Department of Fish and Wildlife, and U.S. Fish and <br />Wildlife Service. <br />centrated mainly on galvanotaxic and electronar- <br />cotic aspects (Taylor et al. 1957; Vibert 1963; Ba- <br />layev 1981). <br />Knowledge of the effects of electricity on fish is <br />important because electrofishing is commonly used <br />to estimate fish population size. Underlying all <br />mark-recapture or depletion estimators are as- <br />sumptions that, if violated, can affect the accuracy <br />of estimates (Gatz and Loar 1988). Of particular <br />importance is the assumption that marked and <br />unmarked fish are equally catchable, which has <br />been the subject of much statistical investigation <br />(Otis et al. 1978; Burnham and Overton 1979; <br />Seber 1982). The failure of this assumption has <br />been reported to affect the accuracy of fish pop- <br />ulation estimates (Cross and Stott 1975; Bohlin <br />and Sundstrom 1977; Peterson and Cederholm <br />1984). If it is assumed that fish must show normal <br />behavior and physiology to meet the assumption <br />of equal catchability, an understanding of the ef- <br />fects of procedures such as electroshocking, han- <br />dling, and marking on physiology and behavior <br />644