Laserfiche WebLink
<br />45 <br /> <br />Northcote (1962) suggested that during times of low light intensity (at night <br />or during periods of high turbidity), fish larvae lose their rheotactic <br />orientation (orientation to a current of water) because of reduced visual <br />orientation and are displaced, usually passively, downstream. Pavlov et ale <br />(1968, 1972) and Girsa (1969) observed that rheotactic disorientation is <br />especially evident in the early larval phases, i.e., protolarvae and <br />mesolarvae. They reported that in early larval teleosts, visual orientation <br />is the only rheotropic mechanism operating and as light intensity decreases, <br />there is a concomitant decrease in the critical velocity at which fish larvae <br />can maintain position in a stream. Conversely, as fish larvae develop, <br />tactile orientation mechanisms begin to function and the importance of visual <br />orientation decreases, hence, rheotropism is possible at reduced light <br />intensities. Further, the ability of fish larvae to maintain position in a <br />stream is directly related to body size and musculature/fin development, <br />hence, smaller individuals exhibit a greater tendency to drift than larger, <br />more developed fish. These observations could explain the observed <br />differences in size (TL) of Colorado squawfish YOY collected in drift-nets <br />versus seines. Only protolarvae and early mesolarvae were collected in <br />drift-nets (size range = 7.2-9.3 rom TL), while larger individuals were <br />collected in seine samples (size range = 8.1-29.0 rom TL). <br /> <br />For some fish, downstream transport of their larvae might be related to <br />feeding habits. Muller (1978) and Armstrong and Brown (1983) hypothesized <br />that fish larvae drift either as a means to obtain food (i.e., maintain <br />association with high densities of drifting food organisms) or as a <br />consequence of feeding (i.e., fish larvae emerge from the substrate to feed <br />and are swept downstream). They and Clark and Pearson (1980) also suggested <br />that downstream transport of fish larvae might be associated with strategies <br />for reducing predator pressure. Gale and Mohr (1978) speculated that <br />drifting fish larvae (especially at night) resemble bits of floating debris; <br />as such they probably are not readily detected by sight-feeding predators. <br /> <br />Finally, Frank and Leggett (1982, 1983), working with capel in larvae <br />(Mallotus villosus) and using ideas in Harper et ale (1961) based on work <br />with plant communities, developed the "safe site" concept as rationale for <br />why fish larvae disperse downstream. "Safe sites" were defined as special <br />areas which provided necessary resources and enhanced individual survival; <br />these areas had high food densities and reduced predation/competition <br />pressures. They reported that depending upon the proximity of "safe sites" <br />to spawning areas, two dispersal strategies were possible: (1) if the <br />probability of finding suitable habitat at a distance from the spawning site <br />was high, then fish larvae were transported downstream into these nursery <br />areas: (2) if the probability of finding suitable habitat locally was high, <br />then the magnitude of larval fish drift was reduced and larvae were retained <br />at or near the spawning area. Nikolsky (1963), Priegal (1970), and Gale and <br />Mohr (1978) observed that a larval drift-dispersal strategy is frequently <br />incorporated into the life cycle of riverine fishes as a means of placing <br />young in adequate nursery/feeding areas. <br /> <br />Tyus (pers. comm.) has reported the collection of larval and juvenile <br />Colorado squawfish from Green River (Utah) backwaters below the Yampa <br />confluence, and it is presumed that some of these individuals originated in <br />the lower Yampa River since no confirmed upstream spawning areas in the Green <br />