<br />:-
<br />
<br />178
<br />
<br />TABlE I.-Number of rainbow and brown trout with
<br />instantaneous growth rates reduced by electroshocking
<br />as a function of number of shockings. Expected values
<br />are in parentheses..
<br />
<br />Number of trout
<br />Nll11Iberof with lower than
<br />ekan>- average growth
<br />shoctings rates
<br />
<br />Number of trout
<br />with greater tban
<br />or equal to
<br />average growth
<br />rates
<br />
<br />Total
<br />number
<br />of trout
<br />
<br />,,;3 29 (32.1) 16 (12.9) 45
<br /><<4 16(12.9) 2(5.1) ]8
<br />
<br />All 45 ] 8 i 63
<br />
<br />. xl - 3.613; df = 1; P < 0.10 for the n\ll hypothesis that the
<br />number of eIectroshockings has no effect on instantaneous growth
<br />rate.
<br />
<br />nilicant. The time elapsed since the most recent
<br />previous electroshocking (i.e., recovery time) did
<br />matter (Table 2). Trout recaptured 1.5-2.5 months
<br />after an earlier electroshocking were significantly
<br />more likely to show below-average growth .'fates
<br />over their entire capture histories than repiatedly
<br />electrosbocked trout that had not been electro-
<br />shocked so recently. Finally, the age of the trout
<br />affected the reduction in growth rate (Table 3).
<br />Age-3+ trout that had been repeatedly e]ectro-
<br />shocked showed no tendency for reduced growth
<br />rates, whereas age-] and age-2 trout had be]ow-
<br />average growth rates 84 and 89% of the time, re-
<br />spectively.
<br />Decreases in growth rates were severe. Of the
<br />'17 trout that showed below average instantaneous
<br />growth rates from among those electrosbocked less
<br />". than 3 months earlier, ] 5 (55%) actually lost weight
<br />"over the 1.5-2.5-month interval between captures.
<br />.Instantaneous growth rates of the other 12 fish in
<br />this category averaged only 51% (range, 3-87%)
<br />oftbe growth rates for all trout of the same species
<br />and age at their respective sites. Although less fre-
<br />quent, reductions in growth could also be severe
<br />among trout with 3 or more months to recover
<br />from electroshocking. Weight losses over the en-
<br />tire time interval between captures occurred in
<br />only three trout with this capture history (17% of
<br />those with reduced growth rates). However, in-
<br />stantaneous growth rates for the other 83% of the
<br />trout with reduced growth rates averaged only 4 ] %
<br />(range, 7-87%) of the growth rates for all trout of
<br />the same species and age at their respective sites.
<br />
<br />Discussion
<br />
<br />Most investigations'on the adverse effects of
<br />e1ectro&bocking on fish have dealt with acute re-
<br />sponses, such as death or physical injuries, caused
<br />
<br />GATZ ET AL.
<br />
<br />TABLE 2.-Number of rainbow and brown trout with
<br />instantaneous growth rates reduced by electroshocking
<br />as a function of time since last capture by shocking.
<br />Expected values are in parentheses..
<br />
<br />Months since Number of trout
<br />previous Number oftrout with greater than
<br />capture by with lower than or equal 10 Total
<br />electro. average growth average growth number
<br />shocking rates rates of trout
<br /><3 21 (22.1) 4 (8.9) 31
<br /><<3 18 (22.9) 14(9.1) 32
<br />All 45 18 63
<br />
<br />. x2 = 1.411; df = I; P < 0.01 for the null hypothesis that
<br />recency of last shocking has no effect on instantaneous growth
<br />rate.
<br />
<br />by single electroshocking events (e.g., Hauck 1949;
<br />Vibert 1963; McCrimmon and Bidgood 1965;
<br />Spencer 1967; Adams et al. 1972; Whaley et al.
<br />1978), although Saul (1980) investigated the ]ethal
<br />effects of repetitive electroshocking. Two studies
<br />have dealt with the effects ofa single electroshock-
<br />ing on reproduction. Maxfield et al. (]971) found
<br />no effect of a single e]ectroshocking on subsequent
<br />fecundity of shocked fish or the survival and growth
<br />of their young, whereas Marriott (1973) reported
<br />a higher mortality in eggs from shocked ripe fe-
<br />males than from unshocked ripe females. The few
<br />earlier studies that have pertained to growth (Hals-
<br />band 1967; Egglishaw 1970; Maxfie]d et a!. 197 I;
<br />Ellis 1974; Kynard and Lonsda]e 1975) found no
<br />effects of e]ectroshocking. Because the results we
<br />report here differ from those of these earlier stud-
<br />ies, a detailed comparison of procedures is desir-
<br />able.
<br />The experimental procedures employed by
<br />Maxfie]d et al. (197]), Ellis (1974), and Kynard
<br />and Lonsdale (1975) were similar to each other
<br />and differed from our study in three major re-
<br />
<br />TABLE 3.-Number of rainbow and brown trout with
<br />instantaneous growth rates reduced by eJectroshocking
<br />as a function of age ofthe trout. Expected values are in
<br />parentheses..
<br />
<br /> Number of trout
<br /> Number of trout with greater
<br /> with lower than than or equal to Total
<br /> average growth average growth number
<br />Age rateS rates oftrout
<br />1 16(13.6) 3 (5.4) 19
<br />2 16 (12.9) 2 (5.1) 18
<br />3+ 13 (]8.6) 13 (1.4) 26
<br />All 45 18 63
<br />
<br />.)(2 = ]0.043; df = 2; P < 0.01 for the null hypothesis that
<br />age of the trout has no effect on instantaneous growth rate.
<br />
<br />
<br />REPEATED ELECTROSHOCKlNG EFfECT ON GROWTH
<br />
<br />179
<br />
<br />spects. In their work, fish were (1) exposed to a
<br />single electroshocking experience, (2) not handled
<br />subsequent to electroshocking, and (3) maintained
<br />and fed under hatchery-like conditions prior to
<br />assessment of the effects of electroshocking on
<br />growth. Al] three of these differences from our
<br />experimental design could contribute to the dis-
<br />similarity of results.
<br />Consider these differences seriatim. First, our
<br />repetition of shocking could be critical. Although
<br />no one has previously studied the effects of re-
<br />petitive shocking on growth, repetition of shocking
<br />does increase the one effect, mortality, that has
<br />been studied (Saul 1980). Second, a general stress
<br />response may have contributed to our growth re-
<br />sults, although previous work offers litt]e support
<br />for this idea. Stress could have been caused by
<br />handling, fin clipping, or MS-222. Pickering et al.
<br />(1982) found no effect of handling on growth rate
<br />but did find that handling decreased feeding by
<br />brown trout for the next 3 d. Their results suggest
<br />that part or all of the short-term 5% average weight
<br />loss reported here may have been a response to
<br />handling stress but not the long-term changes in
<br />growth. Similarly, previous workers have not found
<br />that fin-clipping decreased growth of either hatch-
<br />ery or wild trout (Shetter 1952, 1967; Brynildson
<br />and Brynildson ] 967) even when fin-c]ipping was
<br />combined with the use of MS-222 (Stauffer and
<br />Hansen 1969). This last result does not allow us
<br />to rule out the possibility of an adverse growth
<br />effect in response to multiple exposures to MS-
<br />222. In our work, however, reductions in growth
<br />were similar in 1982 when MS-222 exposure did
<br />not accompany each electroshocking and in 1983
<br />when each electroshocking was accompanied by
<br />MS-222 exposure. Thus, repeated exposure to MS-
<br />222 does not seem to be the cause of the decreased
<br />growth rates we observed. Finally, the high avail-
<br />ability of food under hatchery-like conditions
<br />might mean that only severely weakened fish would
<br />show any decrease in growth rate under the ex-
<br />perimental design used by others, whereas in a
<br />. field study such as ours, the ability to feed, and
<br />I hence also growth, might decrease greatly for even
<br />slightly weakened fish.
<br />The remaining previous study (Egglishaw 1970)
<br />bears more similarity to our own, although one
<br />critical difference exists. Like our work, EggIi-
<br />shaw's was a field study in which salmonids were
<br />subjected to repeated (seven to eight times per
<br />year) e]ectroshockings. The difference is in how
<br />Egglishaw assessed an effect on fish growth. We
<br />calculated growth rates for individual marked fish,
<br />
<br />
<br />~
<br />
<br />whereas Egglishaw compared age-specific mean
<br />weights for fish collected in three study sections
<br />with the corresponding weights for fish collected
<br />from control areas (areas electro fished once a year),
<br />upstream and downstream from the study sites.
<br />This difference in whether or not marked fish were
<br />used has considerable ramifications. Our proce-
<br />dure ensures that individual fish were, in fact, re-
<br />peatedly electroshocked, and that growth rates cal-
<br />cu]ated are for fish with a known history of
<br />electro shocking. In contrast, although Egg]ishaw
<br />repeatedly sampled tbe same sections (27.5 to 30.8
<br />m long) of the stream, neither natural nor exper-
<br />imental barriers prevented movements either in
<br />or out of the study areas after sampling was com-
<br />pleted and the stop-nets were removed. Any fish
<br />that emigrated from the study areas, after having
<br />been sampled one or more times, not only would
<br />have avoided the repeated electroshockings but
<br />also would have been excluded from the growth-
<br />rate calculations for the study section. Instead, pre-
<br />viously unshocked fish could move into the r(TIi-
<br />tively short study sections from elsewhere in the
<br />stream. If such mixing occurred, the uniformity
<br />of growth rates in the control and study areas that
<br />Egglishaw reported is to be expected.
<br />Our own observations suggested that such hy-
<br />pothesized movements were likely. First, 40% of
<br />the trout we captured four or more times moved
<br />between subsections at our study sites. Our sub-
<br />sections averaged nearly three times as long - 8].8
<br />m versus 28.8 m-as EggIishaw's sites so that an
<br />even higher rate of migration might well have oc-
<br />curred there. Second, among all our sites, an av-
<br />erage of 15% of the marked fish emigrated from
<br />the entire study section during just the mark-re-
<br />capture interval of 24-72 h. Again, higher emi-
<br />gration rates might be expected either from smaller
<br />sections or over longer periods of time. Oear]y,
<br />migration of stream-dwelling salmonids out of
<br />short study sections is a phenomenon to be taken
<br />into account, especially in long-term studies. In
<br />the present case, it may well explain why Egglishaw
<br />(1970) found no effect of repeated electroshocking
<br />on growth.
<br />The exact cause of the decrease in growth rates
<br />we observed for repeatedly electrosbocked age-l
<br />and age-2 trout is not known, but it is highly un-
<br />likely that the results were artifacts of our meth-
<br />ods. A nonrandom sampling for slow-growing trout
<br />is unlikely because repeatedly electroshocked trout
<br />averaged the same initial length and weight as the
<br />other trout of the same age at their respective sites.
<br />Spurious results due to changes in the reproductive
<br />
|